Development of Language-specific Influences on Speech Perception and Production in Pre-verbal Infancy

نویسنده

  • Catherine T. Best
چکیده

Adults have difficulty discriminating non-native speech contrasts, yet young infants discriminate both native and nonnative contrasts. Language-specific influences appear by 9-10 months for consonants and phonotactic sequences, and by 6 months for vowels. Early experience is still influential in adulthood, when fluent early bilinguals still show L1 rather than L2 effects on consonant discrimination, and those exposed to a language only during their first year show enhanced sensitivity to its contrasts. Preferences for some native prosodic properties appear even earlier. Newborns prefer native over non-native connected speech, while common native syllable structures and stress patterns are preferred by 6 months. Infant babbling displays similar developmental trends, with native prosodic biases present by 6 months and segmental biases emerging by 10 months. These effects take place prior to true word production, and well before morphology and syntax. Thus, they necessarily occur at a prelexical level. It is hypothesized, then, that language-specific phonetic learning precedes acquisition of contrastive phonology, a concept of fundamental significance to understanding the nature of phonological knowledge and its function in spoken language. 1. ATTUNEMENT TO THE SPEECH ENVIRONMENT Many non-native segmental contrasts pose substantial perceptual difficulties for adult listeners [e.g., 1], although there is systematic variation in degree of difficulty [2,3]. Young infants, by comparison, can discriminate not only the contrasts from their own language environment, but also non-native phonetic contrasts to which they have never been exposed [4, 5, 6]. Converging evidence from numerous laboratories indicates that the developmental reorganization of speech perception implicit in these adult-infant differences has begun before the emergence of the childÕs first words. Specifically, the language environment already exerts a strong impact on infantsÕ discrimination of non-native phonetic contrasts by some time during the second half-year of life [7, 8, 9, 10]. Complementing the perceptual findings are reports that children up to at least 5-6 years of age can easily acquire the phonological properties of a second language (L2) and speak it Òlike a native,Ó whereas most adults speak late-learned second languages with a noticeable accent and continue to show certain perceptual difficulties with L2 consonants and vowels that do not occur in their first language (L1)[11]. These observations suggest that even the youngest children are not listening to speech simply as an end in itself -they are actively engaged in becoming speakers themselves. The phonetic and phonological properties of the ambient language are thus crucial to attunement of speech production as well as perception. So it is perhaps not surprising that native biases also begin to appear in infantsÕ prelinguistic speechlike vocalizations during the second half-year of life. As we might expect, language-specific influences appear earlier in the prosodic properties of infant vocalizations than in their narrower segmental properties [12, 13, 14, 15]. Such language-specific attunement of speech perception and production provide a window on the acquisition of phonological structure in the native language. At the same time, the developmental path of this attunement may also offer unique insights about the underlying nature of phonological knowledge in general, and its function in spoken language. 1.1. Language-specific attunement in speech perception We turn first to review the evidence on language-specific biases in speech perception during infancy. Much research has focused on perception of non-native consonants, as summarized in the first section below. After that we turn to some more recent studies on perception of vowels and broader suprasegmental properties, including prosodic patterns, which offer additional insights regarding organizational levels in the acquisition of the native phonological system. 1.1.1 Consonant perception Discrimination of many non-native consonant contrasts shows a dramatic decline by 10 months of age, with the difficulties starting to emerge between 8-10 months. The first direct evidence of this shift was provided by Werker and colleagues [16, 17, 18]. They found that English-learning 6-8 month olds discriminated not only native /b/-/d/, but also non-native Hindi dental vs. retroflex stops and Nthlakampx velar vs. uvular ejective stops. In sharp contrast, 10-12 month olds failed to discriminate either non-native consonant pair, succeeding only with the native consonants. Hindi and Nthlakampx infants, of course, still discriminated their native contrasts at 10-12 months. That developmental pattern was qualified, though, by findings that English-learning infants discrirminate Zulu click consonants, which are quite different from any English consonants, up to at least 14 months of age [19]. A subsequent study replicated the 10-12 month decline in discrmination of Nthlakampx ejectives, as well as the continued successful discrimination of Zulu clicks, in the same infants [20]. Thus, non-native consonant contrasts vary in difficulty for older infants, as they do for adults [2,3,19]. The finding that click consonants are relatively easy for adults and older infants to discriminate led to the development of the Perceptual Assimilation Model (PAM) of non-native speecvh perception [21, 22, 23]. PAM posits that discrimination depends on the phonetic similarities the listener perceives between the non-native phones and the native phonological system. The model predicts that adults will perceptually assimilate non-native phones to their native phonological system in one of three ways: 1) as categorizable within a native phonological category, where it may either be a near-match for native tokens, a good/acceptable exemplar, or a poor/deviant exemplar; 2) as an uncategorizable speech sound falling into the ÒuncommittedÓ phonetic space in between native categories; 3) as a nonspeech sound falling page 1261 ICPhS99 San Francisco entirely outside native phonological space (i.e., failure of assimilation as a speech segment). Given those possibilities, adultsÕ discrimination of a non-native phonetic contrast will depend on assimilation of each phone. Thus, several pairwise assimilation types are possible, including (but not limited to): 1) assimilation of the contrasting non-native phones to Two Categories (TC) in the native phonology, yielding excellent to very good discrimination; 2) assimilation of both non-native phones equally well (or poorly) to a Single Category (SC), yielding poor discrimination; 3) assimilation of both phones to a single native category, but with a Category Goodness difference (CG) in fit to the category, yielding good discrimination but lower than for TC contrasts; or 4) the non-native phones may be too discrepant from any native categories to be assimilated to the native phonological system, instead being heard as NonAssimilable (NA) nonspeech, yielding fairly good discrimination as non-linguistic sounds. To test the TC, CG and SC predictions, we began by testing American adults on three additional consonant contrasts from Zulu and one from Tigrinya [23,24]. A Zulu lateral fricative voicing distinction and a Tigrinya ejective bilabial vs. dental stop distinction were both expected to be assimilated as TC contrasts with excellent discrimination. A Zulu voiceless aspirated vs. ejective velar stop distinction was expected to yield CG assimilation and good but lower discrimination. Finally, a Zulu plosive vs. implosive voiced bilabial stop distinction was expected to be assimilated as a SC contrast, with poor discrimination. These assimilation and discrimination predictions for adults were strongly upheld. Therefore, we next tested American English-learning 6-8 and 10-12 month olds on the same contrasts [25,26]. All four contrasts were discriminated by 6-8 month olds, as expected. However, the 10-12 month olds failed to discriminate any of the Zulu contrasts, including the lateral fricatives (TC contrast for adults) and the Zulu velars (CG contrast for adults). Nonetheless, the older infants discriminated English /s/-/z/, as well as the Tigrinya ejectives (TC contrast for adults). The results suggest that 10-12 month olds may have less detailed internal phonetic organization of native categories than do adults, as evidenced by their failure to discriminate the Zulu CG velar contrast. This lack of phonetic differentiation may have interferred with theolder infantsÕ discrimination of the non-native fricatives (which adults tended to assimilate as impermissible consonant clusters), even though it did not hamper discrimination of non-native ejectives (which adults assimilated as singletons). Two findings suggest that the impact of such early languagespecific effects on non-native consonant perception may persist into adulthood. First, fluent bilinguals show an L1 rather than an L2 (or combined) effect on non-native consonant discrimination in adulthood even if their L2 was acquired prior to 5 years [27]. Second, exposure to a language during only the first year of life appears to alleviate difficulties in discriminating consonant contrasts from that language in adulthood [28]. 1.1.2. Vowel perception Discrimination of non-native vowels may decline earlier thn discrimination of consonants. A native language impact on vowel perception may already be apparent by 6 months [29], even for some contrasts that adults in the infantÕs language environment can discriminate well [30, cf. 31]. Specifically, English-learning 4-month-olds discriminated two German high frontvs. backrounded vowel contrasts (tense, lax), whereas 10-12 month olds failed to discriminate the same contrasts. By comparison, 6-8 month olds showed an asymmetry in which the more English-like vowel of each pair was discriminated after habituation to the less English-like vowel, but discrimination failed in the converse direction [30]. That pattern appears consistent with the discrimination asymmetry reported by Kuhl and colleagues as a Òperceptual magnet effect,Ó which American and Swedish 6 month olds each demonstrated for native but not for non-native vowels [29]. In related PAM studies with American adults, we found SC assimilation of the Norwegian high-front unrounded vs. rounded vowel contrast, with predictably poor discrimination accuracy. Instead, the Thai midvs. high-back unrounded vowel contrast was assimilated as a CG difference (within English schwa), and were discriminated very well. Three remaining vowel contrasts (French high vs. mid front rounded, French mid-high front vs. central rounded, Norwegian high front unrounded vs. high-central rounded) all showed TC assimilation and excellent discrimination. Finally, the French oral vs. nasal mid-back rounded vowel distinction showed mixed CG/TC assimilation with excellent discrimination [32]. When we tested American English-learning infants with a Norwegian vowel contrast that adults assimilated as a TC type (high front vs. central rounded vowels), we found good discrimination at 3-5 months and a complete lack of discrimination at 10-12 months [33]. But we found a discrimination asymemtry at 6-8 months in which the high front-rounded vowel, which is more peripheral in the vowel space than is the high central-rounded vowel, acted like a perceptual magnet [see 31]. In an ongoing extension of that study, we are testing infantsÕ discrimination of the Norwegian vowel contrast that adults assimilate as a SC type (high front rounded vs. unrounded). The results are complete only for the youngest age, 3-5 months. Intriguingly, even this age group failed to discrimiante this particular contrast, which may be consistent with the fact that these two vowels are equally peripheral in the vowel space [see also 31,34,35]. 1.1.3. Perception of prosodic patterns Infants show even earlier sensitivity to certain broad prosodic properties of the native language, but this tendency fails to extend to all suprasegmental properties, particularly those that reflect language-specific phonological or syntactic organization. For example, on the one hand, even newborns prefer listening to fluent speech in the native language over that in a non-native language [36]. However, it is not until 9 months of age that infants reliably prefer typical over atypical bisyllabic stress patterns in the native language [37]. And only by 11 months do infants reliably prefer native over non-native phonotactic sequences [38] or show sensitivity to word boundaries in fluent native speech [39]. And when presented with native and nonnative prosodic contrasts corresponding to syntactic distinctions, such as the question versus statement distinction, infants surprisingly fail to show language-specific differences in discrimination even as late as 12 months of age [40,41]. Moreover, infants continue to prefer the prosodic patterns of infant-directed speech over adult-directed speech even for unfamiliar non-native languages, until at least 8-10 months [42]. 2.1. Language-specific attunement in speech production Production often lags perception developmentally, proceeding at different rates for different organizational levels. As with speech perception of speech, we expect early language-specific differentiation at prosodic levels, with segmental differentiation page 1262 ICPhS99 San Francisco emerging later. Such a path is compatible with nonsegmental views of child phonology, which suggest the first organizational unit is the syllable, word, or phrase, rather than the phoneme. 2.1.1. Production of prosodic patterns As expected, language-specific influences appear earlier for broader prosodic structures in infant vocalizations than for narrower segmental-level patterns [12,13,14,15]. Certain prosodic properties of the target language are evident in infant babbling as early as 6 months of age [43,44]. For example, the larger proportion of rising than falling intonation for French learners compared with English learners [45] is consistent with this intonational difference in the two languages. Interestingly, babbled vowels at around 6 months of age show cross-language commonality in their display of intrinsic F0 (IF0), the tendency for high vowels like /i/ and /u/ to have higher F0s than low vowels such as /a/. Groups of infants who were learning continental French versus learning American English both showed higher F0s for high vowels in their babbling, suggesting that IF0 is an automatic consequence of articulation, rather than a learned prosodic property [13]. 2.1.2. Vowel and consonant production In contrast, trends toward the native distribution of vowel and consonant categories in infant vocal productions have not been found until around 10 months. At that point, the native-language effect is fairly clear for vowels, but less robust for consonants, though still significant [e.g., 15,46,47]. 2. UNDERLYING NATURE OF EARLY ATTUNEMENT Because these effects of language experience to both speech perception and speech production take place prior to the onset of true word use by children, and certainly well before the onset of productive morphology and syntax, it is hypothesized that the experiential effects occur at the pre-lexical rather than at the lexical (or syntactic) level. Thus, the reorganization of perception and production in infancy most likely reflects language-specific phonetic learning but does not yet indicate knowledge of contrastive phonology. This developmental pattern suggests that the abstract contrastive units of phonology may not themselves be innate, but rather that they must be discovered by the child through active exploration of the physical, superficial phonetic patterns of the speech around them. Children must learn through experience to recognize the systematic relations that exist between the phonetic patterns of the spoken language and the phonological functions they serve in the language, that is, between the physical details of spoken utterances and the abstract linguistic system that gives rise to them. ACKNOWLEDGMENTSWork supported by NIH grants DC00403 and HD01994. REFERENCES[1] Miyawaki, K., Strange, W., Verbrugge, R., Liberman, A. M., Jenkins,J. J., & Fujimura, O. 1975. An effect of linguistic experience: Thediscrimination of [r] and [l] by native speakers of Japanese andEnglish. Perception and Psychophysics, 18, 331-340.[2] Polka, L. 1991. Cross-language speech perception in adults:Phonemic, phonetic, and acoustic contributions. Journal of theAcoustical Society of America, 89, 2961-2977.[3] Best, C. T. 1995. A direct realist perspective on cross-language speechperception. In W. Strange & J. J. Jenkins (eds.), Cross-languagespeech perception Timonium, MD: York Press.[4] Best, C. T. 1984. Discovering messages in the medium: Speech andthe prelinguistic infant. In H. E. Fitzgerald, B. Lester, & M. Yogman(Eds.), Advances in pediatric psychology. New York: Plenum.[5] Werker, J. F., Gilbert, J. H. V., Humphrey, K., & Tees, R. C. 1981.Developmental aspects of cross-language speech perception. ChildDevelopment, 52, 349-355.[6] Trehub. S. E. 1976. The discrimination of foreign speech contrasts byadults and infants. Child Development, 47, 466-472.[7] Werker, J. F. 1989. Becoming a native listener. American Scientist,77, 54-59.[8] Best, C. T. 1994. Learning to perceive the sound pattern of English. InC. Rovee-Collier & L. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancy research.Hillsdale, NJ: Ablex Publishers.[9] Jusczyk, P. W. 1993. From general to language-specific capacities:The WRAPSA model of how speech perception develops. Journal ofPhonetics, 21, 3-28.[10] Kuhl, P. K. 1993. Innate predispositions and the effects of experiencein speech perception: The native language magnet theory. In B. d.Boysson-Bardies, S. d. Schonen, P. Jusczyk, P. MacNeilage, & J.Morton (eds.), Developmental neurocognition: Speech and faceprocessing in the first year of life. Boston: Kluwer Academic.[11] Flege, J. E. 1990. Perception and production: The relevance ofphonetic input to L2 language learning. In C. Ferguson & T. Heubner(Eds.), Crosscurrents in second language acquisition and linguistictheories Philadelphia: John Benjamins.[12] Levitt, A. G., & Wang, Q. 1991. Evidence for language-specificrhythmic influences in the reduplicative babbling of FrenchandEnglish-learning infants. Language and Speech, 34, 235-249.[13] Whalen, D. H., Levitt, A. G., Hsiao, P.-L., & Smorodinsky, I. 1995.Intrinsic F0 of vowels in the babbling of 6-, 9and 12-month-oldFrenchand English-learning infants. Journal of the AcousticalSociety of America, 97, 2533-2539.[14] Oller, D. K. 1980. The emergence of the sounds of speech in infancy.In G. H. Yeni-Komshian, J. F. Kavanagh, & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.),Child phonology:: Production. New York: Academic Press.[15] Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E. 1982. Similarity of babbling in Spanish-and English-learning babies. Journal of Child Language, 9, 565-577.[16] Werker, J. F., Gilbert, J. H. V., Humphrey, K., & Tees, R. C. 1981.Developmental aspects of cross-language speech perception. ChildDevelopment, 52, 349-355.[17] Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. 1984. Cross-language speechperception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the firstyear of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 49-63.[18] Werker, J. F., & Lalonde, C. E. 1988. Cross-language speechperception: Initial capabilities and developmental change.Developmental Psychology, 24, 672-683.[19] Best, C. T., McRoberts, G. W., & Sithole, N. M. 1988. Examinationof perceptual reorganization for nonnative speech contrasts: Zuluclick discrimination by English-speaking adults and infants. Journalof Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4,45-60.[20] Best, C. T., McRoberts, G. W., LaFleur, R., & Silver-Isenstadt, J.1995. Divergent developmental patterns for infants' perception of twonon-native consonant contrasts. Infant Behavior and Development,18, 339-350.[21] Best, C. T. 1994. Learning to perceive the sound pattern of English.In C. Rovee-Collier & L. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancyresearch. Hillsdale, NJ: Ablex Publishers.[22] Best, C. T. 1995. A direct realist perspective on cross-languagespeech perception. In W. Strange & J. J. Jenkins (Eds.), Cross-language speech perception Timonium, MD: York Press.[23] Best, C. T. 1993. Emergence of language-specific constraints inperception of non-native speech: a window on early phonologicaldevelopment. In B. d. Boysson-Bardies, S. de Schonen, P. Jusczyk, P.MacNeilage, & J. Morton (Eds.), Developmental neurocognition:speech and face processing in the first year of life. Dordrecht: KluwerAcademic Publishers.[24] Best, C. T. 1990. Adult perception of nonnative contrasts differing inassimilation to native phonological categories. Journal of the page 1263ICPhS99 San Francisco Acoustical Society of America, 88, S177(A).[25] Best, C. T. 1991. Phonetic influences on the perception of nonnativespeech contrasts by 6-8 and 10-12 month olds. Paper presented at theSociety for Research in Child Development, Seattle.[26] Best, C. T. 1994. Learning to perceive the sound pattern of English.In C. Rovee-Collier & L. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancyresearch. Hillsdale, NJ: Ablex Publishers.[27] Calder—n, J. & Best, C. T. 1996. Effects of bilingualism on theperception of nonnative consonant voicing contrasts. Presented atmeeting of the Acoustical Society of America. Indianapolis.[28] Werker, J.F. & Tees, R. C. 1984. Phonemic and phonetic factors inadult cross-language speech perception. Journal of the AcousticalSociety of America, 75, 1866-1878.[29] Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. N., &Lindblom, B. 1992. Linguistic experience alters phonetic perceptionin infants by 6 months of age. Science, 255, 606-608.[30] Polka, L., & Werker, J. F. 1994. Developmental changes inperception of nonnative vowel contrasts. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 421-435.[31] Polka, L. & Bohn, O.-S. 1996. A cross-language comparison ofvowel perception in English-learning and German-learning infants.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 100, 577-592.[32] Best, C. T., Faber, A., & Levitt, A. G. 1996. Perceptual assimilationof non-native vowel contrasts to the American English vowel system.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 99, S2602.[33] Best, C. T., Singh, L., Bouchard, J., Connelly, G., Cook, A., &Faber, A. 1997. Developmental changes in infantsÕ discrimination ofnon-native vowels that adults assimilate to two native categories.Presented at meeting of the International Society for Infant Studies,Providence.[34] Lacerda, F. 1993. Sonority contrasts dominate young infantsÕ vowelperception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 93, S2372.[35] Lacerda, F. 1994. The asymmetric structure of the infantÕs vowelspace. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 95, S3016[36] Moon, C.; Cooper, R. P., & Fifer, W. P. 1993. Two-day-olds prefertheir native language. Infant Behavior & Development, 16, 495-500.[37] Jusczyk, P. W., Cutler, A., & Redanz, N. J. 1993. Infants' preferencefor the predominant stress patterns of English words. ChildDevelopment, 64, 675-687.[38] Jusczyk, P. W., Friederici, A. D., Wessels, J. M. I., Svenkerud, V.Y., & Jusczyk, A. M. 1993. Infants' sensitivity to the sound patternsof native language words. Journal of Memory and Language, 32,402-420.[39] Myer,s J., Jusczyk, P. W., Kemler Nelson, D. G., Charles-Luce, J.,Woodward, A. L., Hirsh-Pasek, K. 1996. Infants' sensitivity to wordboundaries in fluent speech. Journal of Child Language, 23, 1-30.[40] Best, C. T., Levitt, A. G., & McRoberts, G. W. 1991. Examination oflanguage-specific influences on infantsÕ discrimination of prosodiccategories. Presented at meeting of International Congress ofPhonetic Sciences, Aic-en-Provence.[41] Best, C. T., Hampel, L., & Chiang, S. Y. 1992. Discrimination ofEnglish and Mandarin discourse prosody contrasts by English-learning infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 15 , 311 (A). [42] Best, C. T., Calder—n, J., Jones, C., Avery, R., Correa, L., &McRoberts, G. W. 1997. Preferences of 4-6 and 8-10 month olds forinfant-directed vs. adult-directed utterances in two non-nativelanguages. Presented at meeting of the International Society forInfant Studies, Providence.[43] Levitt, A. G. 1993. The acquisition of prosody: Evidence fromFrenchand English-learning infants. In B. d. Boysson-Bardies, S. deSchonen, P. Jusczyk, P. MacNeilage, & J. Morton (Eds.),Developmental neurocognition: speech and face processing in thefirst year of life.. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.[44] Levitt, A. G., & Wang, Q. 1991. Evidence for language-specificrhythmic influences in the reduplicative babbling of FrenchandEnglish-learning infants. Language and Speech, 34, 235-249.[45] Whalen, D. H., Levitt, A. G., & Wang, Q. 1991. Intonationaldifferences between the reduplicative babbling of FrenchandEnglish-learning infants. Journal of Child Language, 18, 501-516.[46] Boysson-Bardies, B. d., HallŽ, P., Sagart, L., & Durand, C. 1989. Acrosslinguistic investigation of vowel formants in babbling. Journalof Child Language, 16, 1-17.[47] Levitt, A. G., & Aydelott Utman, J. G. 1992. From babbling towardsthe sound systems of English and French: A longitudinal two-casestudy. Journal of Child Language, 19, 19-49. page 1264ICPhS99 San Francisco

برای دانلود رایگان متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

Persian Cued Speech: The Effect on the Perception of Persian Language Phonemes and Monosyllabic Words with and without Sound in Hearing Impaired Children

Objectives: This paper studies the effect of Persian Cued Speech on the perception of Persian language phonemes and monosyllabic words with and without sound in hearing impaired children. Cued Speech is a sound based mode of communication for hearing impaired people that is comprised of a limited series of hand complements and the normal pattern of speech. And it is shown that it effectively ca...

متن کامل

The Role of Sociolinguistics in Second Language Acquisition

Learning a new language also involves learning a broad system of norms for social relations.This study broadly showed how EFL learners’ speech act is conveyed from their nativecultures when they are communicating in English and demonstrated that there are somepossibilities of cross-cultural misunderstanding when interlocutors are engaged in the speechact of complimenting with native speakers of...

متن کامل

Verbal-Auditory Skills in 5-year-Old Children of Semnan/Iran in 2006

Introduction: This research was planned to determine some verbal-auditory skills (verbal-auditory short memory and phonological awareness) that have the closest relationship with speech and language development in 5-year-old children. Method: In this descriptive cross-sectional study, 400 children of pre-school classes affiliated to Education and Welfare organizations in Semnan city were select...

متن کامل

Sensorimotor influences on speech perception in infancy.

The influence of speech production on speech perception is well established in adults. However, because adults have a long history of both perceiving and producing speech, the extent to which the perception-production linkage is due to experience is unknown. We addressed this issue by asking whether articulatory configurations can influence infants' speech perception performance. To eliminate i...

متن کامل

Realization of Verbal Appropriacy/ Inappropriacy in Persian: A Variational Pragmatics approach

Abstract: Avoiding the current terminology debates in the literature on politeness research  and following  a  variational  pragmatics  approach,  this  study  attempted  to  illuminate  how appropriacy/  inappropriacy  is  realized  in  Persian  language  in  light  of  five  speech  acts  of introduction,  apology,  refusal,  congratulation,  and  condolence.  Additionally,  it  was  aimed  t...

متن کامل

Perception Development of Complex Syntactic Construction in Children with Hearing Impairment

Objectives: Auditory perception or hearing ability is critical for children in acquisition of language and speech hence hearing loss has different effects on individuals’ linguistic perception, and also on their functions. It seems that deaf people suffer from language and speech impairments such as in perception of complex linguistic constructions. This research was aimed to study the pe...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 1999